Creativity training for preschool children and its effect on self-regulation
Abstract
Creativity and self-regulation are related skills that develop rapidly in early childhood. This study aims to set up creativity training for preschoolers and then examine the effects of this training on cognitive (i.e. executive functions) and emotional (i.e. emotion-regulation) dimensions of self-regulation. Four -and five-year-old children (n=80) participated in the study. Imagination and symbolic play-based activities were the main elements of the creativity training that lasted for six sessions. Each session was approximately 40 minutes. We used the Alternative/Different Use Task (in the pre-test) and Wallach-Kogan Creativity and the Sampling Tasks (in the post-test) to evaluate creativity performance. Verbal ability was also tested to control. The training boosted creativity in the experiment group. There was an increase from pre-test to post-test in the training group's executive functions and emotion-understanding performances as well. This training suggests a relationship between creativity and self-regulation in terms of cognitive flexibility and emotion comprehension.
References
Anderson, D. R., Huston, A. C., Schmitt, K. L., Linerbarger, D. L., ve Wright, J. C. (2001). Creativity. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66(1), 67-78. doi: 10.1111/1540-5834.00125
Anderson, V. (2001). Assessing executive functions in children: Biological, psychological, and developmental considerations. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 4, 119-136.
Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., ve Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of research on mood and creativity: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779-806.
Beck, D. M., Schaefer, C., Pang, K., ve Carlson, S. M. (2011). Executive Function in Preschool Children: Test-Retest Reliability. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12(2), 169-193.
Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., ve Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(4), 480-485. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.014
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Sommer, M., Arendasy, M., ve Neubauer, A.C. (2014). Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: The common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence, 46, 73-83.
Bogoyavlenskaya, D. B. (2013). Nature of Changes in Creativity Scores in Preschool and Junior Schoolchildren. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 86, 358-362. doi: doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.08.579
Bott, N., Quintin, E. M., Saggar, M., Kienitz, E., Royalty, A., Hong, D. W. C., Liu, N., Chien, Y. H., Hawthorne, G., ve Reiss, A. (2014). Creativity training enhances goal-directed attention and information processing. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 13, 120-128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2014.03.005
Bronson, M. (2000). Self-regulation in early childhood: Nature and nurture. Guilford Press.
Butcher, J. L., ve Niec, L. N. (2005). Disruptive behaviors and creativity in childhood: The ımportance of affect regulation. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 181-193. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2005.9651478
Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 595-616.
Carlson, S. M., ve Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11, 282-298.
Carlson, S. M., ve Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Development, 72(4), 1032-1053.
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., ve Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 499-506. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.499
Cheung, P. C., Lau, S., Chan, D. W., ve Wu, W. Y. H. (2004). Creative potential of school children in Hong Kong: Norms of the Wallach-Kogan Creativity Tests and their implications. Creativity Research Journal, 16(1), 69-78. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj1601_7
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., ve Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development, 75, 317-333.
Cutting, A., ve Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: Individual differences and interrelations. Child Development, 70(4), 853-865.
Dansky, J. L. (1980). Make-believe: A mediator of the relationship between play and associative fluency. Child Development, 51, 576-579.
De Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A., Baas, M., Wolsink, I. ve Roskes, M. (2012). Working memory benefits creative ınsight, musical improvisation, and original ideation through maintained task-focused attention. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(5), 656-669. doi: 10.1177/0146167211435795
De Rosnay, M., ve Harris, P. L. (2002). Individual differences in children's understanding of emotion: The role of attachment. Attachment and Human Development, 4(1), 39-54. doi: 10.1080/14616730210123139
Dempster, F. N. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Developmental Review, 12, 45-75.
Denham, S. A., Ferrier, D. E., Howarth, G. Z., Herndon, K. J., ve Bassett, H. (2016). Key considerations in assessing young children's emotional competence. Cambridge Journal of Education, 46(3), 299-317.
Diamond, A. (2016). Why improving and assessing executive functions early in life is critical. J. A. Griin, P. McCardle, ve L. S. Freund (Eds.) içinde. Executive function in preschool age children: Integrating measurement, neuro development and translational research (ss. 11-38). Washington: American Psychological Association.
Dillon, J. A. (2009). Play, creativity, emotion regulation and executive functioning. (Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis). Case Western Reserve University, USA.
Duckworth, A. L., ve Steinberg, L. (2015). Unpacking self-control. Child Development Perspectives, 9(1), 32-37.
Dziedziewicz, D., ve Karwowski, M. (2015). Development of children's creative visual imagination: A theoretical model and enhancement programmes, Education 3-13, 43(4), 382-392. doi: 10.1080/03004279.2015.1020646
Edl, S., Benedek, M., Papousek, I., Weiss, E.M., ve Fink, A. (2014). Creativity and the stroop interference effect. Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 38-42.
Eisenberg, N., Sadovsky, A., ve Spinrad, T. L. (2005). Associations of emotion-related regulation with language skills, emotion knowledge, and academic outcomes. New Directions For Child And Adolescent Development, 109, 109-118.
Elias, C. L., ve Berk, L. E. (2002). Self‐regulation in young children: Is there a role for sociodramatic play? Early Child Research Quarterly, 17, 216-231. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885‐2006(02)00146‐1
Fehr, K. K., ve Russ, S. W. (2016). Pretend play and creativity in preschool-age children: Associations and brief intervention. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10(3), 296-308. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000054
Fein, G. G. (1987). Pretend Play: Creativity and Consciousness. D. Görlitz, ve J. F. (Eds). İçinde Curiosity, Imagination, and Play: On the Development of Spontaneous Cognitive and Motivational Processes (ss. 281–304). US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Förster, J., ve Dannenberg, L. (2010). GLOMOsys: A systems account of global versus local processing. Psychological Inquiry, 21, 175-197. doi:10.1080/1047840X.2010.487849
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., ve Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development, 10(4), 483-527. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(95)90024-1
Fuhs, M. W., ve Day, J. D. (2011). Verbal ability and executive functioning development in preschoolers at Head Start. Developmental Psychology, 47, 404-416.
Galyer, K. T., ve Evans, J. M. (2001). Pretend play and the development of emotion regulation in preschool children. Early Child Development and Care, 166, 93-108. https://doi.org/10.1080/0300443011660108
Garaigordobil, M. (2006). Intervention in creativity with children aged 10 and 11 years: Impact of a play program on verbal and graphic–figural creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 18(3), 329-345. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj1803_8
Garaigordobil, M., ve Berrueco, L. (2011). Effects of a play program on creative thinking of preschool children. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14(2), 608-618. http://dx.doi.org/10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14.n2.9
Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., ve Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 3 1/2-7 years old on a Stroop-like Day-Night Test. Cognition, 53(2), 129-153.
Gilpin, A. T., Brown, M. M., ve Pierucci, J. M. (2015). Relations between fantasy orientation and emotion regulation in preschool. Early Education and Development, 26(7), 920-932. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2015.1000716
Golden, C. J. (1975). The measurement of creativity by the Stroop Color and Word Test. Journal of Personality Assessment, 39, 502-506. doi: 10.1207/s 15327752jpa3905_9
Groborz, M., ve Necka, E. (2003). Creativity and cognitive control: Explorations of generation and evaluation skills. Creativity Research Journal, 15, 183-197. doi:10.1207/S15326934CRJ152ve3_09
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5(9), 444-454. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0063487
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Hoffmann, J. D., ve Russ, S. W. (2016). Fostering pretend play skills and creativity in elementary school girls: A group play intervention. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10(1), 114-125. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000039
Hoffmann, J., ve Russ, S. (2012). Pretend play, creativity, and emotion regulation in children. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(2), 175-184. doi: 10.1037/a0026299
Holmes, R. M., Romeo, L., Ciraola, S., ve Grushko, M. (2015). The relationship between creativity, social play, and children's language abilities. Early Child Development and Care, 185(7), 1180-1197. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2014.983916
Hresko, W. P., Reid, D. K., ve Hammill, D. D. (1999). Test of early language development (teld) third edition. Texas: PRO-ED.
Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., ve Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77(6), 1822-1841.
Johnson, L. D. (1985). Creative thinking potential: another example of U-shaped development? Creative Child ve Adult Quarterly, 10, 146-159.
Jurado, M. B., ve Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: A review of our current understanding. Neuropsychology Review, 17, 213-233. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z
Kim, S., Nordling, J. K., Yoon, J. E., Boldt, L. J. ve Kochanska, G. (2013). Effortful control in "hot" and "cool" tasks differentially predicts children's behavior problems and academic performance. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 43-56. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9661-4
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 220-232.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L., ve Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. Child Development, 67(2), 490-507.
Lau, S., ve Cheung, P. C. (2010). Creativity assessment: Comparability of the electronic and paper-and-pencil versions of the Wallach–Kogan Creativity Tests. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 5(3), 101-107.
Leung, A. K. Y., Qiu, L., Chiu, C. Y., Liou, S., Kwan, L. Y. Y., ve Yong, J. C. (2014). The role of ınstrumental emotion regulation in the emotions–creativity link: How worries render ındividuals with high neuroticism more creative. Emotion, 14(5), 846-856. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036965
Liebermann, D., Giesbrecht, G. F., ve Müller, U. (2007). Cognitive and emotional aspects of self-regulation in preschoolers. Cognitive Development, 22, 511-529.
Lindsey, E. W., ve Colwell, M. J. (2003). Preschoolers' emotional competence: Links to pretend play and physical play. Child Study Journal, 33, 39-52.
Matthews, S. B. (2008). The relationship among self‐regulation, sociodramatic play, and preschoolers' readiness for kindergarten. (Unpublished Ph.D Thesis). Northeastern University, Boston.
Mcclelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Duncan, R., Bowles, R. P., Acock, A. C., Miao, A., ve Pratt, M. E. (2014). Predictors of early growth in academic achievement: The Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders Task. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 599. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00599
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., ve Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "frontal lobe" tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100.
Montroy, J. J., Bowles, R. P., Skibbe, L. E., McClelland, M. M., ve Morrison, F. J. (2016). The development of self-regulation across early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 52(11), 1744-1762.
Moore, M., ve Russ, S. W. (2008). Follow-up of a pretend play intervention: Effects on play, creativity, and emotional processes in children. Creativity Research Journal, 20(4), 427-436. doi: 10.1080/10400410802391892
Mottweiler, C. M., ve Taylor, M. (2014). Elaborated role play and creativity in preschool age children. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(3), 277-286. doi: 10.1037/a0036083
Nader‐ Grosbois, N., ve Vieillevoye, S. (2012). Variability of self‐regulatory strategies in children with intellectual disability and typically developing children in pretend play situations. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 56, 140-156. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365‐2788.2011.01443.x
Nijstad, B. A., De Dreu., C. K. W., Rietzschel, E. F., ve Baas, M. (2010). Towards a dual-pathway to creativity model: Creative ideation as a function of flexibility and persistence. European Review of Social Psychology, 21, 34-77. doi:10.1080/10463281003765323
Nusbaum, E. C., ve Silvia, P. J. (2011). Are intelligence and creativity really so different? Fluid intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use in divergent thinking. Intelligence, 39, 36-45. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2010.11.002
Peker, H. (2013). Anaokulu çocuklarında benlik düzenleme ve otonominin yaratıcılık üzerindeki etkisi. (Yayımlanmamış Yüksek Lisans Tezi). Mersin Üniversitesi, Mersin.
Pennington, B. F., ve Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51-87.
Ponitz, C. C., McClelland, M. M., Matthews, J. S., ve Morrison, F. J. (2009). A structured observation of behavioral self-regulation and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 605-619. doi: 10.1037/a0015365.
Pons, F., Harris, P. L., ve De Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: Developmental periods and hierarchical organization. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1(2), 127-152. doi: 10.1080/17405620344000022
Radel, R., Davranche, K., Fournier, M., ve Dietrich, A. (2015). The role of (dis)inhibition in creativity: Decreased inhibition improves idea generation. Cognition, 134, 110-120. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.001
Richard, R. (1990). Everyday creativity, eminent creativity, and health: Afterview for CRT ıssues on creativity and health. Creativity Research Journal, 3, 300-326.
Runco, M. A. (2014). Creativity: Theories and themes: Research, development, and practice. USA: Elsevier Academic Press.
Runco, M. A., ve Charles, R. E. (1993). Judgments of originality and appropriateness as predictors of creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 537-546. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(93)90337-3
Russ, S. W. (2014). Pretend play in childhood: Foundation of adult creativity. USA: American Psychological Association.
Russ, S. W., ve Wallace, C. E. (2013). Pretend play and creative processes. American Journal of Play, 6(1), 136-148.
Russ, S. W., Robins, A. L., ve Christiano, B. A. (1999). Pretend play: Longitudinal prediction of creativity and affect in fantasy in children. Creativity Research Journal 12, 129-139.
Sagone, E., ve De Caroli, M. E. (2014a). Emotion comprehension and divergent thinking: What's their relationship in developmental age? Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 116, 585-589. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.01.261
Sagone, E., ve De Caroli, M. E. (2014b). The relationship between emotion comprehension and mental synthesis in developmental age. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 116, 590-595. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.01.262
Sharma, S., ve Babu, N. (2017). Interplay between creativity, executive function and working memory in middle-aged and older adults. Creativity Research Journal, 29(1), 71-77. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2017.1263512
Shennan, S. (2001). Demography and cultural innovation: A model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 11, 5-16.
Shields, A., ve Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among school-age children: The development and validation of a new criterion q-sort scale. Developmental Psychology, 33(6), 906-916.
Sowden, P. T., Clements, L., Redlich, C., ve Lewis, C. (2015). Improvisation facilitates divergent thinking and creativity: Realizing a benefit of primary school arts education. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9(2), 128-138. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000018
Topbaş, S. ve Güven, S. (2012). Türkçe Erken Dil Gelişimi Testi kullanım klavuzu. Ankara: Detay Yayıncılık.
Torrance, E. P. (1965). Scientific views of creativity and factors affecting its growth. Creativity and Learning, 94(3), 663-681. http://www.jstor.org/stable/20026936
Torrance, E. P. (1966). Torrance tests of creative thinking- norms technical manual research edition- verbal tests, forms A and B- figural tests, forms A and B. Princeton: Personnel Pres. Inc.
Vieillevoye, S., ve Nader‐Grosbois, N. (2008). Self‐regulation during pretend play in children with intellectual disability and in normally developing children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 29, 256-272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2007.05.003
Vygotsky, L. S. (1967a). Imagination and creativity in childhood. Journal of Russian and Eastern European Psychology, 42(1), 7-97.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1967b). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. Soviet Psychology, 5, 6-18.
Wallace, C. (2013). Pretend play and resiliency: A follow-up study of school-aged children. (Unpublished Master's thesis). Case Western Reserve University, US.
Wallach, M. A., ve Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children a study of the creativity intelligence distinction. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, Inc.
Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., ve Groisser, D. B. (1991). A normative-developmental study of executive function: A window on prefrontal function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, 131-149.
Yeh, Y. C., ve Li, M. L. (2008). Age, emotion regulation strategies, temperament, creative drama, and preschoolers' creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 42, 131-149. doi:10.1002/j.21626057.2008.tb01291.x
Yeh, Y., ve Lin, C. F. (2015). Aptitude-treatment interactions during creativity training in e-learning: How meaning-making, self-regulation, and knowledge management influence creativity. Educational Technology ve Society, 18(1), 119-131.
Zabelina, D. L., ve Robinson, M. D. (2010). Creativity as flexible cognitive control. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(3), 136-143.
Zelazo, P. D., Carter, A., Reznick, J. S., ve Frye, D. (1997). Early development of executive function: A problem-solving framework. Review of General Psychology, 1, 1-29.
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
Copyright (c) 2025 Journal of Early Childhood Studies

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.